The words you are searching are inside this book. To get more targeted content, please make full-text search by clicking here.

Old Age Psychiatrist (2015) 62 Depressive pseudodementia 6 on whether pseudodementia is a different entity in all of the respects explored, and a clear

Discover the best professional documents and content resources in AnyFlip Document Base.
Search
Published by , 2016-01-12 00:03:03

Depressive pseudodementia – how ‘pseudo’ is it really?

Old Age Psychiatrist (2015) 62 Depressive pseudodementia 6 on whether pseudodementia is a different entity in all of the respects explored, and a clear

Old Age Psychiatrist (2015) 62 Depressive pseudodementia

Depressive pseudodementia – how ‘pseudo’ is it really?

Dr James Kennedy
FY1 Doctor, Care of the Elderly, North Bristol NHS Trust, Southmead Hospital, Bristol
Email: [email protected]

Abstract: Depressive pseudodementia is a term commonly used to describe a condition
whereby a patient experiences a cognitive deficit secondary to a primary mood disorder. This
essay will explore whether depressive pseudodementia can truly be described as a dementia
mimic or not. It argues that pseudodementia is closer in clinical nature to ‘true’ dementia, is
not fully reversible, may act as a prodrome for ‘true’ dementia, and has an organic basis. All
of these factors suggest that the term pseudodementia is misplaced, and the condition is more
closely related to dementia than previously appreciated.

Introduction

Pseudodementia is a widely debated term in psychiatry, for which a consensus on definition
is difficult to find. Most sources use it as a descriptive term for a cognitive deficit secondary
to a primary psychiatric disturbance which mimics dementia, and which is reversible once the
primary diagnosis has been treated. Such primary conditions include major depressive
disorder, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and Ganser’s syndrome.

I will focus on depressive pseudodementia, resulting from a unipolar or bipolar affective
disorder, which makes up the vast majority of pseudodementia described in the literature. I
will explore whether we can consider depressive pseudodementia to be truly reversible,
whether it acts as a risk factor or a prodrome for developing later dementia, and whether there
are neuropathological changes which may explain the condition. I will then discuss the
consequences of these findings, and address whether the term pseudodementia is outmoded.

Defining depressive pseudodementia

The term pseudodementia entered the psychiatric lexicon in 1961 with Kiloh’s landmark
paper on the topic, but descriptions of the condition are found much earlier [1]. Albert Mariet
(1852-1935), Professor of Mental Illness in the University of Montpellier published Démence
Mélancholique (his treatise on ‘melancholic dementia’) in 1883 [2]. Since then, there has
been a steady interest in the topic, particularly in the past decade owing to the widespread use
of imaging techniques which have enabled us to seek an organic substrate for the condition.

Kiloh’s paper emphasised that the term should not be used diagnostically, just descriptively;
he saw it as hugely important to identify patients with reversible causes of cognitive
impairment, so they are not confined to the “therapeutic nihilism” of being labelled with an
irreversible untreatable disease [3]. His criteria were as follows:

(i) pseudodementia is an intellectual impairment in a patient with a psychiatric disorder;
(ii) the intellectual deficit is reversible;
(iii)the features resemble those of a neuropathologically induced cognitive deficit;
(iv)there is no apparent underlying neuropathological process.

The difficulty in defining pseudodementia relates to the polemic surrounding its exact nature.
The word itself implies that the cognitive deficit is not a ‘true’ dementia, which is defined by
the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual 4th Edition of the American Psychiatric Association
(DSM-IV) as being ‘the development of multiple cognitive deficits (including memory

1

Old Age Psychiatrist (2015) 62 Depressive pseudodementia

impairment) that are due to [an organic cause]’ [4]. DSM-IV mentions pseudodementia
fleetingly in the context of distinguishing dementia from a major depressive episode using a
patient’s premorbid state, stating that if there is a long history of premorbid cognitive decline
dementia is more likely, as opposed to a sudden decline associated with depression which is
more likely to be a pseudodementia.

Kiloh’s criteria somewhat distance pseudodementia from the DSM-IV definition of dementia.
Yet several studies have shown that features we consider in the realm of dementia –
irreversibility, progression and neuropathological changes – are also found in patients with
depressive pseudodementia.

Nature of pseudodementia

In 1979, Wells described what he saw as the key differences in presentation between
pseudodementia and dementia [5]. Features more consistent with pseudodementia than
organic dementia included a precise onset, rapid deterioration, relatively good attention and
concentration, patchy memory loss, and minimal patient effort to cope with the dysfunction.
He was quite clear that pseudodementia ‘caricatures’ rather than ‘mimics’ dementia, and that
his criteria could be used to distinguish clinically between the conditions.

Yet in practice it is not that simple to establish a definitive diagnosis. Many features overlap,
and the complex relationship between depression and dementia and their frequent coexistence
in the older population makes it hard to discern which the primary diagnosis is. This
confusion is also found in the literature. Tobe describes a case report highlighting just how
difficult it is to disentangle the diagnoses of depression and dementia, in particular when
confounded with polypharmacy, and in this case, differing views and lack of collaboration
between clinicians [6].

All in all, it is fair to say that though Wells’ criteria may have helped distinguish
pseudodementia from dementia in his patient set, the differences between pseudodementia
and dementia are not that well demarcated.

Is it reversible?

Classically one of the major distinguishing features between dementia and pseudodementia is
that of reversibility. Depressive pseudodementia is considered to be a key reversible form of
cognitive deficit, whereby if the affective disorder at play is treated, so is the dementia [7].
Nonetheless, a lot of evidence would suggest that even once a patient’s mood has been
stabilised the patient may still have residual cognitive impairment, albeit with less
impairment than during the period of pseudodementia. We will examine a selection here.

Saez-Fonseca looked into the long-term consequences of depressive pseudodementia [8]. As
well as finding a fourfold increase in relative risk for developing dementia in the years
following the episode of pseudodementia (discussed below), it was interesting to note that the
Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) score was still significantly lower in the ‘reversible’
cognitive impairment group after treatment compared to the group of non-cognitively
impaired depressed patients. This suggests there is still residual cognitive impairment in the
pseudodementia group, and thus it is not fully reversible. The mean Geriatric Depression
Scale (GDS) score after treatment was significantly higher in the pseudodementia group too;
the authors interpreted this as patients with pseudodementia having depression more resistant
to treatment, but even if this the case we are unable to tell from this study whether, if the
GDS score fully returned to normal, the MMSE would also return fully to baseline.

A 2013 study used electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) to treat 20 patients with depressive
pseudodementia, and found that it resolved both symptoms of depression and dementia [9].

2

Old Age Psychiatrist (2015) 62 Depressive pseudodementia

Vast improvements in MMSE and depression scores were seen, although notably over half
(11/20) patients still had an MMSE score of below 27/30 (considered to be the cut-off for
normal cognition) after 8 weeks of treatment, despite their mood post-treatment returning to
within normal range according to the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD).

Altogether, although pseudodementia is classically considered a reversible form of cognitive
impairment, evidence would suggest that this is not necessarily the case.

Prodrome or risk factor ... or both?

One of the key debates in the field of depressive pseudodementia is whether depressive
episodes, with or without coexisting cognitive impairment, either act as an independent risk
factor for the development of dementia later in life, or represent a prodromal state to the
dementia. The difference is based partly on the time interval between the onset of depression
and the onset of dementia (an association significantly before the onset of dementia would
imply depression being a risk factor rather than a prodrome), as well as whether there is a
period free of cognitive impairment between the depressive episode and the onset of
dementia (if not, that would imply a prodrome).

The case for depressive symptoms being a prodrome for dementia has been widely made, for
example in one key community based prospective study by Chen, which found that
depressive symptoms were prodromal to dementia rather than predictive of it [10].
Schweitzer reviewed the evidence and concluded that the epidemiological data support
depression as a prodrome for both Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia, but there is
still controversy as to whether it is a true independent risk factor [11]. Confusion arises as to
the huge heterogeneity of study types that have been used to examine the relationship.
Though the study population did not have cognitive testing done during the depressive
episode to confirm pseudodementia, Bhalla found a twice increased risk of mild cognitive
impairment immediately after resolution of depressive symptoms in patients diagnosed with
late life depression, suggesting the depression and cognitive impairment may be part of the
same disease process [12].

On the other hand, one meta-analysis containing 20 studies and over 100,000 subjects
reported that depression doubled the risk of developing dementia later in life [13]. The
authors suggested depressive episodes acted as an independent risk factor rather than a
prodrome (though they do not rule out both being a possibility) as the interval between
diagnoses is positively and significantly correlated with risk of developing Alzheimer’s
disease. This agreed with another key study in the field, which found that, though the
relationship was strongest when onset of depression was closer in time to onset of dementia,
the strength of the relationship over one year and even over 25 years apart suggested that it
was unlikely to just be a prodrome, but also an independent risk factor [14].

Kessing and Anderson performed a very large longitudinal study (22,974 patients) over 29
years from 1970 to 1999 looking at whether prior admissions to a psychiatric ward for
depressive disorder or bipolar disorder were related to subsequent readmission for dementia
[15]. It reported that the rate of dementia increased by 13% for every episode leading to
admission for depressive disorder, and 6% for bipolar disorder. Although this study did not
look at pseudodementia specifically, and may have underestimated dementia diagnosed
elsewhere than on a psychiatric ward, it was a powerful study and lends weight to the theory
that depression is a risk factor for dementia later in life. In addition, Saez-Fonseca reported a
relative risk of nearly 4 for patients admitted for depression who presented with
pseudodementia, compared with those who were cognitively intact, for developing dementia
at follow up 5-7 years later [8].

3

Old Age Psychiatrist (2015) 62 Depressive pseudodementia

Perhaps the distinction between prodrome and risk factor is a false one: late life depression
could be both a prodrome and a risk factor for dementia; they need not be mutually exclusive
[16]. Kasahara built six models into which cases in the literature fall; three where depressive
episodes precede the dementia by some time (more in keeping with depression as a risk
factor), one of which includes a period of pseudodementia during the depressive phase, and
three where depression is a precursor symptom, an initial symptom, or it just appears during
the course of the dementia [17].

Kobayashi and Kato propose that pseudodementia and depression in Alzheimer’s disease lie
on a spectrum from depression without dementia to dementia without depression [18]. They
established this in the context of Janet’s concept of ‘psychological tension’, whereby
depression lowers psychological tension revealing latent dementia. They named the concept
the ‘depression-dementia medius’ [19]. They saw pseudodementia as a revelation of latent
dementia in depressive illness, and go as far as suggesting that treatments useful for
depressive symptoms may be useful in treating those with dementia even if they do not
exhibit depressive symptoms simply by reducing psychological tension (for example, by
providing a reassuring environment, prescribing antidepressants, using music and
reminiscence therapies, or even ECT).

So what can we draw from this? The evidence that depression acts as a risk factor for
dementia is fascinating, and stresses even further the importance of prevention and early
treatment of mood disorders, especially later in life. Yet the studies that suggest it may be a
prodrome are particularly relevant to our discussion, as it adds a new dimension to the
pseudodementia debate – could depressive pseudodementia sit somewhere within this disease
process linking late life depression and dementia? We may be seeing evidence that
pseudodementia is anything but ‘pseudo’, but actually a precursor to ‘true’ dementia.
Regarding implications for current services, this highlights one of the issues with having
memory clinics separate from community mental health teams with little interaction, in that if
depression and dementia are so intrinsically linked, addressing them separately may not be
providing optimum care for patients.

Proposed biological mechanisms for depressive pseudodementia

The biological mechanism of pseudodementia and the organic links between depression and
dementia merit exploration. In the 19th century Marriet observed that there were changes to
the temporal lobe post mortem in patients with depression [2]. His small series of relatively
young patients (21 cases with a mean age of 41 years) nonetheless showed no significant
difference in overall extent and nature of depressive symptoms and cognitive deficit when
compared to a ‘current’ series of patients with pseudodementia from 1985. Marriet believed
melancholic dementia to be organic in nature, due to the temporal relationship between the
melancholia and the ‘weakening of the intelligence’. Several studies have since suggested
organic changes in patients with pseudodementia, although sometimes the areas involved
differ slightly from those involved in classical dementias.

Dolan performed positron emission tomography (PET) scans of patients with major
depressive disorder and reversible cognitive impairment, and found blood flow abnormalities
in anatomical areas additional to those seen in depression and distinct from those seen in
neurodegenerative dementia (namely decreases in the left anterior medial prefrontal cortex
(PFC) and increases in the cerebellar vermis) [20]. Though the authors caution against using
their techniques clinically to differentiate between ‘organic’ dementia and depressive
pseudodementia, they claim that they demonstrate an anatomical role for the PFC and vermis
in depressive pseudodementia.

4

Old Age Psychiatrist (2015) 62 Depressive pseudodementia

Caine reviewed the evidence of memory testing in depressive pseudodementia patients and
found that patients with depressive pseudodementia exhibit a ‘subcortical’ pattern of memory
loss, with patterns of cognitive impairment being similar to patients with subcortical
dementias such as Huntington’s Disease (except for the learning of new information, in
which depressive pseudodementia patients perform significantly better) [21]. A later study by
Massmann corroborates this, finding that patterns of memory loss determined using the
California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) in patients with depressive pseudodementia are
more similar to patients with subcortical dementia than with cortical dementia [22].

Butters suggested various biological substrates as to how the link between depression and
cognitive impairment could be mediated [16]. Hippocampal volume loss might result from
impaired hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal central drive in depression, leading to chronically
elevated glucocorticoid levels and resultant hippocampal neural damage. Also,
cerebrovascular disease and Alzheimer’s disease pathology may interact to cause
frontostriatal damage and hippocampal volume loss, causing simultaneous depression and
mild cognitive impairment (MCI) which then progresses over time to Alzheimer’s disease or
vascular dementia. Other sources propose that the high prevalence of deep white matter
lesions found in various brain imaging studies of patients with depression might link it
aetiologically to vascular dementia [11].

Overall, though there are no conclusive findings as to the nature and location of anatomical
changes in depressive pseudodementia, the likelihood is that they are there, and may be quite
similar in nature to the changes found in several types of dementia. This again would
contribute to the argument that pseudodementia is more akin to dementia than it is currently
credited with.

Conclusion – is pseudodementia an appropriate term?

It seems that, based on work in the field to date, depressive pseudodementia may be closer to
‘true’ dementia than previously considered. An array of evidence suggests that
pseudodementia is often difficult to distinguish from dementia clinically, it may not be
entirely reversible, it may act as a prodrome to dementia, and it may have an organic basis
with structural brain changes. Thus, we must pose the question: is pseudodementia truly
‘pseudo’? And if not, should the term pseudodementia be dropped altogether as misleading
and inaccurate?

I would argue that the evidence presented in this essay suggests that pseudodementia shares
enough features with organic dementia for the prefix ‘pseudo’ to be erroneous. Snowdon’s
review of Kiloh’s landmark paper suggests that only the few cases with truly reversible
dementia and no lasting cognitive impairment can be called pseudodementia, and if there are
organic changes in pseudodementia the prefix is misplaced [3]. However, he does conclude
that the term is still useful in instigating debate into the aetiology behind a particular patient’s
cognitive impairment.

In terms of renaming the condition, Reifler suggests that pseudodementia might best be
renamed predementia, due to the weight of evidence suggesting it acts as a prodrome to
dementia [23]. Perhaps even ‘pseudopseudodementia’, though unwieldy, would be more
accurate for the majority of cases of pseudodementia which have an organic basis and are not
fully reversible.

Whichever term we use to describe the condition, it remains that depressive pseudodementia
is of considerable relevance. Mood disorders are a major issue in old age psychiatry, and the
complex relationship between mood disturbance and cognitive impairment is gradually
becoming elucidated. It would be fair to say not enough research has yet explicitly focussed

5

Old Age Psychiatrist (2015) 62 Depressive pseudodementia

on whether pseudodementia is a different entity in all of the respects explored, and a clear
judgement on how pseudodementia should be considered clinically should be reserved until
this is carried out. A deeper understanding of the phenomenon of depressive pseudodementia
would help clinicians best treat the condition and allow for more accurate prognoses to be
formed.

This essay was awarded third place in the University of Oxford Medical School
Catherine Oppenheimer Prize for Old Age Psychiatry.

References

1. Kiloh, L. G. Pseudo-dementia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 37:336-351 (1961)

2. Berrios, G. E. “Depressive pseudodementia” or “Melancholic dementia”: a 19th century view.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 48:393-400 (1985)

3. Snowdon, J. Pseudodementia, a term for its time: The impact of Leslie Kiloh's 1961 Paper.
Australas Psychiatry 19:391 (2011)

4. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Fourth Edition. American Psychiatric
Association. (1994)

5. Wells, C. E. Pseudodementia. Am J Psychiatry 136:895-900. (1979)

6. Tobe, E. Pseudodementia caused by severe depression. BMJ Case Reports Published online:
14 December 2012

7. Muangpaisan, W., Petcharat, C. and Srinonprasert, V. Prevalence of potentially reversible
conditions in dementia and mild cognitive impairment in a geriatric clinic. Geriatr Gerontol
Int 12:59-64. (2012)

8. Saez-Fonseca, J.A., Lee L., Walker Z. Long-term outcome of depressive pseudodementia in
the elderly. J Affect Disord 101:123-129 (2007)

9. Rapinesi, C., Serata, D., Del Casale, A. Kotzalidis, G. D., Mazzarini, L., Fensore, C.,
Carbonetti, P., Scatena, P., Capezzuto, S., Moscati, F. M., Brugnoli, R., Tatarelli R., Girardi,
P. Depressive pseudodementia in the elderly: effectiveness of electroconvulsive therapy. Int J
Geriatr Psychiatry 28:433-440. (2013)

10. Chen, P., Ganguli, M., Mulsant, B. H. The temporal relationship between depressive
symptoms and dementia: a community-based prospective study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56:261-
266. (1999)

11. Schweitzer, I., Tuckwell, V., O'Brien, J., Ames, D. Is late onset depression a prodrome to
dementia? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 17:997-1005. {2002)

12. Bhalla, R. K., Butters, M. A., Becker, J. T., Houck, P. R., Snitz, B. E., Lopez, O. L., Reynolds
III, C. F. Patterns of mild cognitive impairment after treatment of depression in the elderly.
Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 17:308-316. (2009)

13. Ownby, R. L., Crocco, E., Acevedo, A. Depression and risk for Alzheimer disease:
systematic review, meta-analysis, and metaregression analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63:530-
538. (2006)

14. Green, R.C., Cupples, L.A., Kurz, A., Auerbach, S., Go R., Sadovnick, D., Duara, R., Kukull,
W.A., Chui, H., Edeki, T., Griffith, P.A., Friedland, R.P., Bachman, D,. Farrer, L. Depression
as a risk factor for Alzheimer disease: the MIRAGE Study. Arch Neurol 60:753-759 (2003)

6

Old Age Psychiatrist (2015) 62 Depressive pseudodementia

15. Kessing, L.V., Anderson, P.K. Does the risk of developing dementia increase with the
number of episodes in patients with depressive disorder and in patients with bipolar disorder?
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 75:1662-1666 (2004)

16. Butters, M. A., Young, J. B., Lopez, O., Aizenstein, H. J., Mulsant, B. H., Reynolds, C. F.,
Becker, J. T. Pathways linking late-life depression to persistent cognitive impairment and
dementia. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 10:345-357 (2008)

17. Kasahara, H., Tsumura, M., Ochiai, Y., Furukawa, H., Aoki, K., Ito, T., Kada, H.,
Hashidume, T., Nakanishi, T., Consideration of the relationship between depression and
dementia. Psychogeriatrics 6:128-133 (2006)

18. Kobayashi, T. and Kato, S. Depression–dementia medius: Between depression and the
manifestation of dementia symptoms. Psychogeriatrics 11:177-82. (2011)

19. Janet, P. La Force et la Faiblesse Psychologiques. Paris: Éditions Medicales Norbert Maloine
(1932)

20. Dolan, R.J., Bench, C.J., Brown, R.G., Scott, L.C., Friston, K.J., Frackowiak, R.S.J. Regional
cerebral blood flow abnormalities in depressed patients with cognitive impairment. J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry 55:768-773 (1992)

21. Caine, E. Pseudodementia: Current concepts and future directions. Arch Gen Psychiatry
38:1359-1364 (1981)

22. Massman, P.J., Delis, D.C., Butters, N., Dupont R.M., Gillin, J.C. The subcortical dysfunction
hypothesis of memory deficits in depression: Neuropsychological validation in a subgroup of
patients. J Clin Exp Neuropsyc 14: 687-706 (1992)

23. Reifler, B.V. A case of mistaken identity: pseudodementia is really predementia. J Am
Geriatr Soc 48: 593-594 (2000)

7


Click to View FlipBook Version